Web
and Book design, |
Click
Here to return to |
CHAPTER V. LAWS OF VARIATION. Effects of external conditions—Use and disuse, combined with natural selection; organs of flight and of vision—Acclimatisation—Correlation of growth—Compensation and economy of growth—False correlations—Multiple, rudimentary, and lowly organised structures variable—Parts developed in an unusual manner are highly variable: specific characters more variable than generic: secondary sexual characters variable—Species of the same genus vary in an analogous manner—Reversions to long lost characters—Summary. I HAVE
hitherto sometimes spoken as if the variations—so common and multiform in
organic beings under domestication, and in a lesser degree in those in a state
of nature—had been due to chance. This, of course, is a wholly incorrect
expression, but it serves to acknowledge plainly our ignorance of the cause of
each particular variation. Some authors believe it to be as much the function
of the reproductive system to produce individual differences, or very slight
deviations of structure, as to make the child like its parents. But the much
greater variability, as well as the greater frequency of monstrosities, under
domestication or cultivation, than under nature, leads me to believe that
deviations of structure are in some way due to the nature of the conditions of
life, to which the parents and their more remote ancestors have been exposed
during several generations. I have remarked in the first chapter—but a long
catalogue of facts which cannot be here given would be necessary to show the
truth of the remark—that the reproductive system is eminently susceptible to
changes in the conditions of life; and to this system being functionally
disturbed in the parents, I chiefly attribute the varying or plastic condition
of the offspring. The male and female sexual elements seem to be affected
before that union takes place which is to form a new being. In the case of
“sporting” plants, the bud, which in its earliest condition does not apparently
differ essentially from an ovule, is alone affected. But why, because the
reproductive system is disturbed, this or that part should vary more or less,
we are profoundly ignorant. Nevertheless, we can here and there dimly catch a
faint ray of light, and we may feel sure that there must be some cause for each
deviation of structure, however slight. How much
direct effect difference of climate, food, &c., produces on any being is
extremely doubtful. My impression is, that the effect is extremely small in the
case of animals, but perhaps rather more in that of plants. We may, at least,
safely conclude that such influences cannot have produced the many striking and
complex co-adaptations of structure between one organic being and another,
which we see everywhere throughout nature. Some little influence may be
attributed to climate, food, &c.: thus, E. Forbes speaks confidently that
shells at their southern limit, and when living in shallow water, are more
brightly coloured than those of the same species further north or from greater
depths. Gould believes that birds of the same species are more brightly
coloured under a clear atmosphere, than when living on islands or near the
coast. So with insects, Wollaston is convinced that residence near the sea
affects their colours. Moquin-Tandon gives a list of plants which when growing
near the sea-shore have their leaves in some degree fleshy, though not
elsewhere fleshy. Several other such cases could be given. The fact
of varieties of one species, when they range into the zone of habitation of
other species, often acquiring in a very slight degree some of the characters
of such species, accords with our view that species of all kinds are only
well-marked and permanent varieties. Thus the species of shells which are
confined to tropical and shallow seas are generally brighter-coloured than
those confined to cold and deeper seas. The birds which are confined to
continents are, according to Mr. Gould, brighter-coloured than those of
islands. The insect-species confined to sea-coasts, as every collector knows,
are often brassy or lurid. Plants which live exclusively on the sea-side are
very apt to have fleshy leaves. He who believes in the creation of each
species, will have to say that this shell, for instance, was created with
bright colours for a warm sea; but that this other shell became bright-coloured
by variation when it ranged into warmer or shallower waters. When a
variation is of the slightest use to a being, we cannot tell how much of it to
attribute to the accumulative action of natural selection, and how much to the
conditions of life. Thus, it is well known to furriers that animals of the same
species have thicker and better fur the more severe the climate is under which
they have lived; but who can tell how much of this difference may be due to the
warmest-clad individuals having been favoured and preserved during many
generations, and how much to the direct action of the severe climate? for it
would appear that climate has some direct action on the hair of our domestic
quadrupeds. Instances
could be given of the same variety being produced under conditions of life as
different as can well be conceived; and, on the other hand, of different
varieties being produced from the same species under the same conditions. Such
facts show how indirectly the conditions of life must act. Again, innumerable
instances are known to every naturalist of species keeping true, or not varying
at all, although living under the most opposite climates. Such considerations
as these incline me to lay very little weight on the direct action of the
conditions of life. Indirectly, as already remarked, they seem to play an
important part in affecting the reproductive system, and in thus inducing
variability; and natural selection will then accumulate all profitable
variations, however slight, until they become plainly developed and appreciable
by us. Effects of
Use and Disuse.—From the facts alluded to in the first chapter, I
think there can be little doubt that use in our domestic animals strengthens
and enlarges certain parts, and disuse diminishes them; and that such
modifications are inherited. Under free nature, we can have no standard of
comparison, by which to judge of the effects of long-continued use or disuse,
for we know not the parent-forms; but many animals have structures which can be
explained by the effects of disuse. As Professor Owen has remarked, there is no
greater anomaly in nature than a bird that cannot fly; yet there are several in
this state. The logger-headed duck of South America can only flap along the
surface of the water, and has its wings in nearly the same condition as the
domestic Aylesbury duck. As the larger ground-feeding birds seldom take flight
except to escape danger, I believe that the nearly wingless condition of
several birds, which now inhabit or have lately inhabited several oceanic
islands, tenanted by no beast of prey, has been caused by disuse. The ostrich
indeed inhabits continents and is exposed to danger from which it cannot escape
by flight, but by kicking it can defend itself from enemies, as well as any of
the smaller quadrupeds. We may imagine that the early progenitor of the ostrich
had habits like those of a bustard, and that as natural selection increased in
successive generations the size and weight of its body, its legs were used
more, and its wings less, until they became incapable of flight. Kirby has
remarked (and I have observed the same fact) that the anterior tarsi, or feet,
of many male dung-feeding beetles are very often broken off; he examined
seventeen specimens in his own collection, and not one had even a relic left.
In the Onites apelles the tarsi are so habitually lost, that the insect has
been described as not having them. In some other genera they are present, but
in a rudimentary condition. In the Ateuchus or sacred beetle of the Egyptians,
they are totally deficient. There is not sufficient evidence to induce us to
believe that mutilations are ever inherited; and I should prefer explaining the
entire absence of the anterior tarsi in Ateuchus, and their rudimentary
condition in some other genera, by the long-continued effects of disuse in
their progenitors; for as the tarsi are almost always lost in many dung-feeding
beetles, they must be lost early in life, and therefore cannot be much used by
these insects. In some
cases we might easily put down to disuse modifications of structure which are
wholly, or mainly, due to natural selection. Mr. Wollaston has discovered the
remarkable fact that 200 beetles, out of the 550 species inhabiting Madeira,
are so far deficient in wings that they cannot fly; and that of the twenty-nine
endemic genera, no less than twenty-three genera have all their species in this
condition! Several facts, namely, that beetles in many parts of the world are
very frequently blown to sea and perish; that the beetles in Madeira, as
observed by Mr. Wollaston, lie much concealed, until the wind lulls and the sun
shines; that the proportion of wingless beetles is larger on the exposed Dezertas
than in Madeira itself; and especially the extraordinary fact, so strongly
insisted on by Mr. Wollaston, of the almost entire absence of certain large
groups of beetles, elsewhere excessively numerous, and which groups have habits
of life almost necessitating frequent flight;—these several considerations have
made me believe that the wingless condition of so many Madeira beetles is
mainly due to the action of natural selection, but combined probably with
disuse. For during thousands of successive generations each individual beetle
which flew least, either from its wings having been ever so little less
perfectly developed or from indolent habit, will have had the best chance of
surviving from not being blown out to sea; and, on the other hand, those beetles
which most readily took to flight will oftenest have been blown to sea and thus
have been destroyed. The
insects in Madeira which are not ground-feeders, and which, as the
flower-feeding coleoptera and lepidoptera, must habitually use their wings to
gain their subsistence, have, as Mr. Wollaston suspects, their wings not at all
reduced, but even enlarged. This is quite compatible with the action of natural
selection. For when a new insect first arrived on the island, the tendency of
natural selection to enlarge or to reduce the wings, would depend on whether a
greater number of individuals were saved by successfully battling with the
winds, or by giving up the attempt and rarely or never flying. As with mariners
shipwrecked near a coast, it would have been better for the good swimmers if
they had been able to swim still further, whereas it would have been better for
the bad swimmers if they had not been able to swim at all and had stuck to the
wreck. The eyes
of moles and of some burrowing rodents are rudimentary in size, and in some
cases are quite covered up by skin and fur. This state of the eyes is probably
due to gradual reduction from disuse, but aided perhaps by natural selection.
In South America, a burrowing rodent, the tuco-tuco, or Ctenomys, is even more
subterranean in its habits than the mole; and I was assured by a Spaniard, who
had often caught them, that they were frequently blind; one which I kept alive
was certainly in this condition, the cause, as appeared on dissection, having
been inflammation of the nictitating membrane. As frequent inflammation of the
eyes must be injurious to any animal, and as eyes are certainly not
indispensable to animals with subterranean habits, a reduction in their size
with the adhesion of the eyelids and growth of fur over them, might in such
case be an advantage; and if so, natural selection would constantly aid the
effects of disuse. It is well
known that several animals, belonging to the most different classes, which
inhabit the caves of Styria and of Kentucky, are blind. In some of the crabs
the foot-stalk for the eye remains, though the eye is gone; the stand for the
telescope is there, though the telescope with its glasses has been lost. As it
is difficult to imagine that eyes, though useless, could be in any way
injurious to animals living in darkness, I attribute their loss wholly to
disuse. In one of the blind animals, namely, the cave-rat, the eyes are of
immense size; and Professor Silliman thought that it regained, after living
some days in the light, some slight power of vision. In the same manner as in
Madeira the wings of some of the insects have been enlarged, and the wings of
others have been reduced by natural selection aided by use and disuse, so in
the case of the cave-rat natural selection seems to have struggled with the
loss of light and to have increased the size of the eyes; whereas with all the
other inhabitants of the caves, disuse by itself seems to have done its work. It is
difficult to imagine conditions of life more similar than deep limestone
caverns under a nearly similar climate; so that on the common view of the blind
animals having been separately created for the American and European caverns,
close similarity in their organisation and affinities might have been expected;
but, as Schiodte and others have remarked, this is not the case, and the
cave-insects of the two continents are not more closely allied than might have
been anticipated from the general resemblance of the other inhabitants of North
America and Europe. On my view we must suppose that American animals, having
ordinary powers of vision, slowly migrated by successive generations from the
outer world into the deeper and deeper recesses of the Kentucky caves, as did
European animals into the caves of Europe. We have some evidence of this
gradation of habit; for, as Schiodte remarks, “animals not far remote from
ordinary forms, prepare the transition from light to darkness. Next follow
those that are constructed for twilight; and, last of all, those destined for
total darkness.” By the time that an animal had reached, after numberless
generations, the deepest recesses, disuse will on this view have more or less
perfectly obliterated its eyes, and natural selection will often have effected
other changes, such as an increase in the length of the antennæ or palpi, as a
compensation for blindness. Notwithstanding such modifications, we might expect
still to see in the cave-animals of America, affinities to the other
inhabitants of that continent, and in those of Europe, to the inhabitants of
the European continent. And this is the case with some of the American
cave-animals, as I hear from Professor Dana; and some of the European
cave-insects are very closely allied to those of the surrounding country. It
would be most difficult to give any rational explanation of the affinities of
the blind cave-animals to the other inhabitants of the two continents on the
ordinary view of their independent creation. That several of the inhabitants of
the caves of the Old and New Worlds should be closely related, we might expect
from the well-known relationship of most of their other productions. Far from
feeling any surprise that some of the cave-animals should be very anomalous, as
Agassiz has remarked in regard to the blind fish, the Amblyopsis, and as is the
case with the blind Proteus with reference to the reptiles of Europe, I am only
surprised that more wrecks of ancient life have not been preserved, owing to
the less severe competition to which the inhabitants of these dark abodes will
probably have been exposed. Acclimatisation.—Habit is
hereditary with plants, as in the period of flowering, in the amount of rain
requisite for seeds to germinate, in the time of sleep, &c., and this leads
me to say a few words on acclimatisation. As it is extremely common for species
of the same genus to inhabit very hot and very cold countries, and as I believe
that all the species of the same genus have descended from a single parent, if
this view be correct, acclimatisation must be readily effected during
long-continued descent. It is notorious that each species is adapted to the
climate of its own home: species from an arctic or even from a temperate region
cannot endure a tropical climate, or conversely. So again, many succulent
plants cannot endure a damp climate. But the degree of adaptation of species to
the climates under which they live is often overrated. We may
infer this from our frequent inability to predict whether or not an imported
plant will endure our climate, and from the number of plants and animals
brought from warmer countries which here enjoy good health. We have reason to
believe that species in a state of nature are limited in their ranges by the
competition of other organic beings quite as much as, or more than, by
adaptation to particular climates. But whether or not the adaptation be
generally very close, we have evidence, in the case of some few plants, of
their becoming, to a certain extent, naturally habituated to different
temperatures, or becoming acclimatised: thus the pines and rhododendrons,
raised from seed collected by Dr. Hooker from trees growing at different
heights on the Himalaya, were found in this country to possess different
constitutional powers of resisting cold. Mr. Thwaites informs me that he has
observed similar facts in Ceylon, and analogous observations have been made by
Mr. H. C. Watson on European species of plants brought from the Azores to
England. In regard to animals, several authentic cases could be given of
species within historical times having largely extended their range from warmer
to cooler latitudes, and conversely; but we do not positively know that these
animals were strictly adapted to their native climate, but in all ordinary
cases we assume such to be the case; nor do we know that they have subsequently
become acclimatised to their new homes. As I
believe that our domestic animals were originally chosen by uncivilised man
because they were useful and bred readily under confinement, and not because
they were subsequently found capable of far-extended transportation, I think
the common and extraordinary capacity in our domestic animals of not only
withstanding the most different climates but of being perfectly fertile (a far
severer test) under them, may be used as an argument that a large proportion of
other animals, now in a state of nature, could easily be brought to bear widely
different climates. We must not, however, push the foregoing argument too far,
on account of the probable origin of some of our domestic animals from several
wild stocks: the blood, for instance, of a tropical and arctic wolf or wild dog
may perhaps be mingled in our domestic breeds. The rat and mouse cannot be
considered as domestic animals, but they have been transported by man to many
parts of the world, and now have a far wider range than any other rodent,
living free under the cold climate of Faroe in the north and of the Falklands
in the south, and on many islands in the torrid zones. Hence I am inclined to
look at adaptation to any special climate as a quality readily grafted on an
innate wide flexibility of constitution, which is common to most animals. On
this view, the capacity of enduring the most different climates by man himself
and by his domestic animals, and such facts as that former species of the
elephant and rhinoceros were capable of enduring a glacial climate, whereas the
living species are now all tropical or sub-tropical in their habits, ought not
to be looked at as anomalies, but merely as examples of a very common
flexibility of constitution, brought, under peculiar circumstances, into play. How much
of the acclimatisation of species to any peculiar climate is due to mere habit,
and how much to the natural selection of varieties having different innate
constitutions, and how much to both means combined, is a very obscure question.
That habit or custom has some influence I must believe, both from analogy, and
from the incessant advice given in agricultural works, even in the ancient
Encyclopædias of China, to be very cautious in transposing animals from one
district to another; for it is not likely that man should have succeeded in
selecting so many breeds and sub-breeds with constitutions specially fitted for
their own districts: the result must, I think, be due to habit. On the other
hand, I can see no reason to doubt that natural selection will continually tend
to preserve those individuals which are born with constitutions best adapted to
their native countries. In treatises on many kinds of cultivated plants,
certain varieties are said to withstand certain climates better than others:
this is very strikingly shown in works on fruit trees published in the United
States, in which certain varieties are habitually recommended for the northern,
and others for the southern States; and as most of these varieties are of
recent origin, they cannot owe their constitutional differences to habit. The
case of the Jerusalem artichoke, which is never propagated by seed, and of
which consequently new varieties have not been produced, has even been
advanced—for it is now as tender as ever it was—as proving that acclimatisation
cannot be effected! The case, also, of the kidney-bean has been often cited for
a similar purpose, and with much greater weight; but until some one will sow,
during a score of generations, his kidney-beans so early that a very large
proportion are destroyed by frost, and then collect seed from the few
survivors, with care to prevent accidental crosses, and then again get seed
from these seedlings, with the same precautions, the experiment cannot be said
to have been even tried. Nor let it be supposed that no differences in the
constitution of seedling kidney-beans ever appear, for an account has been
published how much more hardy some seedlings appeared to be than others. On the
whole, I think we may conclude that habit, use, and disuse, have, in some
cases, played a considerable part in the modification of the constitution, and
of the structure of various organs; but that the effects of use and disuse have
often been largely combined with, and sometimes overmastered by, the natural
selection of innate differences. Correlation
of Growth.—I mean by this expression that the whole organisation is
so tied together during its growth and development, that when slight variations
in any one part occur, and are accumulated through natural selection, other
parts become modified. This is a very important subject, most imperfectly
understood. The most obvious case is, that modifications accumulated solely for
the good of the young or larva, will, it may safely be concluded, affect the
structure of the adult; in the same manner as any malconformation affecting the
early embryo, seriously affects the whole organisation of the adult. The
several parts of the body which are homologous, and which, at an early embryonic
period, are alike, seem liable to vary in an allied manner: we see this in the
right and left sides of the body varying in the same manner; in the front and
hind legs, and even in the jaws and limbs, varying together, for the lower jaw
is believed to be homologous with the limbs. These tendencies, I do not doubt,
may be mastered more or less completely by natural selection: thus a family of
stags once existed with an antler only on one side; and if this had been of any
great use to the breed it might probably have been rendered permanent by
natural selection. Homologous
parts, as has been remarked by some authors, tend to cohere; this is often seen
in monstrous plants; and nothing is more common than the union of homologous
parts in normal structures, as the union of the petals of the corolla into a
tube. Hard parts seem to affect the form of adjoining soft parts; it is
believed by some authors that the diversity in the shape of the pelvis in birds
causes the remarkable diversity in the shape of their kidneys. Others believe
that the shape of the pelvis in the human mother influences by pressure the
shape of the head of the child. In snakes, according to Schlegel, the shape of
the body and the manner of swallowing determine the position of several of the
most important viscera. The nature
of the bond of correlation is very frequently quite obscure. M. Is. Geoffroy
St. Hilaire has forcibly remarked, that certain malconformations very
frequently, and that others rarely coexist, without our being able to assign any
reason. What can be more singular than the relation between blue eyes and
deafness in cats, and the tortoise-shell colour with the female sex; the
feathered feet and skin between the outer toes in pigeons, and the presence of
more or less down on the young birds when first hatched, with the future colour
of their plumage; or, again, the relation between the hair and teeth in the
naked Turkish dog, though here probably homology comes into play? With respect
to this latter case of correlation, I think it can hardly be accidental, that
if we pick out the two orders of mammalia which are most abnormal in their
dermal coverings, viz. Cetacea (whales) and Edentata (armadilloes, scaly
ant-eaters, &c.), that these are likewise the most abnormal in their teeth. I know of
no case better adapted to show the importance of the laws of correlation in
modifying important structures, independently of utility and, therefore, of
natural selection, than that of the difference between the outer and inner
flowers in some Compositous and Umbelliferous plants. Every one knows the
difference in the ray and central florets of, for instance, the daisy, and this
difference is often accompanied with the abortion of parts of the flower. But,
in some Compositous plants, the seeds also differ in shape and sculpture; and
even the ovary itself, with its accessory parts, differs, as has been described
by Cassini. These differences have been attributed by some authors to pressure,
and the shape of the seeds in the ray-florets in some Compositæ countenances
this idea; but, in the case of the corolla of the Umbelliferæ, it is by no
means, as Dr. Hooker informs me, in species with the densest heads that the
inner and outer flowers most frequently differ. It might have been thought that
the development of the ray-petals by drawing nourishment from certain other
parts of the flower had caused their abortion; but in some Compositæ there is a
difference in the seeds of the outer and inner florets without any difference
in the corolla. Possibly, these several differences may be connected with some
difference in the flow of nutriment towards the central and external flowers:
we know, at least, that in irregular flowers, those nearest to the axis are
oftenest subject to peloria, and become regular. I may add, as an instance of
this, and of a striking case of correlation, that I have recently observed in
some garden pelargoniums, that the central flower of the truss often loses the
patches of darker colour in the two upper petals; and that when this occurs,
the adherent nectary is quite aborted; when the colour is absent from only one
of the two upper petals, the nectary is only much shortened. With
respect to the difference in the corolla of the central and exterior flowers of
a head or umbel, I do not feel at all sure that C. C. Sprengel’s idea that the
ray-florets serve to attract insects, whose agency is highly advantageous in
the fertilisation of plants of these two orders, is so far-fetched, as it may
at first appear: and if it be advantageous, natural selection may have come
into play. But in regard to the differences both in the internal and external
structure of the seeds, which are not always correlated with any differences in
the flowers, it seems impossible that they can be in any way advantageous to
the plant: yet in the Umbelliferæ these differences are of such apparent
importance—the seeds being in some cases, according to Tausch, orthospermous in
the exterior flowers and cœlospermous in the central flowers,—that the elder De
Candolle founded his main divisions of the order on analogous differences.
Hence we see that modifications of structure, viewed by systematists as of high
value, may be wholly due to unknown laws of correlated growth, and without
being, as far as we can see, of the slightest service to the species. We may
often falsely attribute to correlation of growth, structures which are common
to whole groups of species, and which in truth are simply due to inheritance;
for an ancient progenitor may have acquired through natural selection some one
modification in structure, and, after thousands of generations, some other and
independent modification; and these two modifications, having been transmitted
to a whole group of descendants with diverse habits, would naturally be thought
to be correlated in some necessary manner. So, again, I do not doubt that some
apparent correlations, occurring throughout whole orders, are entirely due to
the manner alone in which natural selection can act. For instance, Alph. De
Candolle has remarked that winged seeds are never found in fruits which do not
open: I should explain the rule by the fact that seeds could not gradually
become winged through natural selection, except in fruits which opened; so that
the individual plants producing seeds which were a little better fitted to be
wafted further, might get an advantage over those producing seed less fitted
for dispersal; and this process could not possibly go on in fruit which did not
open. The elder
Geoffroy and Goethe propounded, at about the same period, their law of
compensation or balancement of growth; or, as Goethe expressed it, “in order to
spend on one side, nature is forced to economise on the other side.” I think
this holds true to a certain extent with our domestic productions: if
nourishment flows to one part or organ in excess, it rarely flows, at least in
excess, to another part; thus it is difficult to get a cow to give much milk
and to fatten readily. The same varieties of the cabbage do not yield abundant
and nutritious foliage and a copious supply of oil-bearing seeds. When the
seeds in our fruits become atrophied, the fruit itself gains largely in size
and quality. In our poultry, a large tuft of feathers on the head is generally
accompanied by a diminished comb, and a large beard by diminished wattles. With
species in a state of nature it can hardly be maintained that the law is of
universal application; but many good observers, more especially botanists,
believe in its truth. I will not, however, here give any instances, for I see
hardly any way of distinguishing between the effects, on the one hand, of a
part being largely developed through natural selection and another and
adjoining part being reduced by this same process or by disuse, and, on the
other hand, the actual withdrawal of nutriment from one part owing to the
excess of growth in another and adjoining part. I suspect,
also, that some of the cases of compensation which have been advanced, and
likewise some other facts, may be merged under a more general principle,
namely, that natural selection is continually trying to economise in every part
of the organisation. If under changed conditions of life a structure before
useful becomes less useful, any diminution, however slight, in its development,
will be seized on by natural selection, for it will profit the individual not
to have its nutriment wasted in building up an useless structure. I can thus
only understand a fact with which I was much struck when examining cirripedes,
and of which many other instances could be given: namely, that when a cirripede
is parasitic within another and is thus protected, it loses more or less
completely its own shell or carapace. This is the case with the male Ibla, and
in a truly extraordinary manner with the Proteolepas: for the carapace in all
other cirripedes consists of the three highly-important anterior segments of
the head enormously developed, and furnished with great nerves and muscles; but
in the parasitic and protected Proteolepas, the whole anterior part of the head
is reduced to the merest rudiment attached to the bases of the prehensile
antennæ. Now the saving of a large and complex structure, when rendered
superfluous by the parasitic habits of the Proteolepas, though effected by slow
steps, would be a decided advantage to each successive individual of the
species; for in the struggle for life to which every animal is exposed, each
individual Proteolepas would have a better chance of supporting itself, by less
nutriment being wasted in developing a structure now become useless. Thus, as I
believe, natural selection will always succeed in the long run in reducing and
saving every part of the organisation, as soon as it is rendered superfluous,
without by any means causing some other part to be largely developed in a
corresponding degree. And, conversely, that natural selection may perfectly
well succeed in largely developing any organ, without requiring as a necessary
compensation the reduction of some adjoining part. It seems
to be a rule, as remarked by Is. Geoffroy St. Hilaire, both in varieties and in
species, that when any part or organ is repeated many times in the structure of
the same individual (as the vertebræ in snakes, and the stamens in polyandrous
flowers) the number is variable; whereas the number of the same part or organ,
when it occurs in lesser numbers, is constant. The same author and some
botanists have further remarked that multiple parts are also very liable to
variation in structure. Inasmuch as this “vegetative repetition,” to use Prof.
Owen’s expression, seems to be a sign of low organisation; the foregoing remark
seems connected with the very general opinion of naturalists, that beings low
in the scale of nature are more variable than those which are higher. I presume
that lowness in this case means that the several parts of the organisation have
been but little specialised for particular functions; and as long as the same
part has to perform diversified work, we can perhaps see why it should remain
variable, that is, why natural selection should have preserved or rejected each
little deviation of form less carefully than when the part has to serve for one
special purpose alone. In the same way that a knife which has to cut all sorts
of things may be of almost any shape; whilst a tool for some particular object
had better be of some particular shape. Natural selection, it should never be
forgotten, can act on each part of each being, solely through and for its
advantage. Rudimentary
parts, it has been stated by some authors, and I believe with truth, are apt to
be highly variable. We shall have to recur to the general subject of
rudimentary and aborted organs; and I will here only add that their variability
seems to be owing to their uselessness, and therefore to natural selection
having no power to check deviations in their structure. Thus rudimentary parts
are left to the free play of the various laws of growth, to the effects of
long-continued disuse, and to the tendency to reversion. A part
developed in any species in an extraordinary degree or manner, in comparison with
the same part in allied species, tends to be highly variable.—Several
years ago I was much struck with a remark, nearly to the above effect,
published by Mr. Waterhouse. I infer also from an observation made by Professor
Owen, with respect to the length of the arms of the ourang-outang, that he has
come to a nearly similar conclusion. It is hopeless to attempt to convince any
one of the truth of this proposition without giving the long array of facts
which I have collected, and which cannot possibly be here introduced. I can
only state my conviction that it is a rule of high generality. I am aware of
several causes of error, but I hope that I have made due allowance for them. It
should be understood that the rule by no means applies to any part, however unusually
developed, unless it be unusually developed in comparison with the same part in
closely allied species. Thus, the bat’s wing is a most abnormal structure in
the class mammalia; but the rule would not here apply, because there is a whole
group of bats having wings; it would apply only if some one species of bat had
its wings developed in some remarkable manner in comparison with the other
species of the same genus. The rule applies very strongly in the case of
secondary sexual characters, when displayed in any unusual manner. The term,
secondary sexual characters, used by Hunter, applies to characters which are
attached to one sex, but are not directly connected with the act of
reproduction. The rule applies to males and females; but as females more rarely
offer remarkable secondary sexual characters, it applies more rarely to them.
The rule being so plainly applicable in the case of secondary sexual
characters, may be due to the great variability of these characters, whether or
not displayed in any unusual manner—of which fact I think there can be little
doubt. But that our rule is not confined to secondary sexual characters is
clearly shown in the case of hermaphrodite cirripedes; and I may here add, that
I particularly attended to Mr. Waterhouse’s remark, whilst investigating this
Order, and I am fully convinced that the rule almost invariably holds good with
cirripedes. I shall, in my future work, give a list of the more remarkable
cases; I will here only briefly give one, as it illustrates the rule in its
largest application. The opercular valves of sessile cirripedes (rock
barnacles) are, in every sense of the word, very important structures, and they
differ extremely little even in different genera; but in the several species of
one genus, Pyrgoma, these valves present a marvellous amount of
diversification: the homologous valves in the different species being sometimes
wholly unlike in shape; and the amount of variation in the individuals of
several of the species is so great, that it is no exaggeration to state that
the varieties differ more from each other in the characters of these important
valves than do other species of distinct genera. As birds
within the same country vary in a remarkably small degree, I have particularly
attended to them, and the rule seems to me certainly to hold good in this
class. I cannot make out that it applies to plants, and this would seriously
have shaken my belief in its truth, had not the great variability in plants
made it particularly difficult to compare their relative degrees of
variability. When we
see any part or organ developed in a remarkable degree or manner in any
species, the fair presumption is that it is of high importance to that species;
nevertheless the part in this case is eminently liable to variation. Why should
this be so? On the view that each species has been independently created, with
all its parts as we now see them, I can see no explanation. But on the view
that groups of species have descended from other species, and have been
modified through natural selection, I think we can obtain some light. In our
domestic animals, if any part, or the whole animal, be neglected and no
selection be applied, that part (for instance, the comb in the Dorking fowl) or
the whole breed will cease to have a nearly uniform character. The breed will
then be said to have degenerated. In rudimentary organs, and in those which
have been but little specialised for any particular purpose, and perhaps in
polymorphic groups, we see a nearly parallel natural case; for in such cases
natural selection either has not or cannot come into full play, and thus the
organisation is left in a fluctuating condition. But what here more especially
concerns us is, that in our domestic animals those points, which at the present
time are undergoing rapid change by continued selection, are also eminently
liable to variation. Look at the breeds of the pigeon; see what a prodigious
amount of difference there is in the beak of the different tumblers, in the
beak and wattle of the different carriers, in the carriage and tail of our
fantails, &c., these being the points now mainly attended to by English
fanciers. Even in the sub-breeds, as in the short-faced tumbler, it is
notoriously difficult to breed them nearly to perfection, and frequently individuals
are born which depart widely from the standard. There may be truly said to be a
constant struggle going on between, on the one hand, the tendency to reversion
to a less modified state, as well as an innate tendency to further variability
of all kinds, and, on the other hand, the power of steady selection to keep the
breed true. In the long run selection gains the day, and we do not expect to
fail so far as to breed a bird as coarse as a common tumbler from a good
short-faced strain. But as long as selection is rapidly going on, there may
always be expected to be much variability in the structure undergoing
modification. It further deserves notice that these variable characters,
produced by man’s selection, sometimes become attached, from causes quite
unknown to us, more to one sex than to the other, generally to the male sex, as
with the wattle of carriers and the enlarged crop of pouters. Now let us
turn to nature. When a part has been developed in an extraordinary manner in
any one species, compared with the other species of the same genus, we may
conclude that this part has undergone an extraordinary amount of modification,
since the period when the species branched off from the common progenitor of
the genus. This period will seldom be remote in any extreme degree, as species
very rarely endure for more than one geological period. An extraordinary amount
of modification implies an unusually large and long-continued amount of
variability, which has continually been accumulated by natural selection for
the benefit of the species. But as the variability of the
extraordinarily-developed part or organ has been so great and long-continued
within a period not excessively remote, we might, as a general rule, expect
still to find more variability in such parts than in other parts of the
organisation, which have remained for a much longer period nearly constant. And
this, I am convinced, is the case. That the struggle between natural selection
on the one hand, and the tendency to reversion and variability on the other
hand, will in the course of time cease; and that the most abnormally developed
organs may be made constant, I can see no reason to doubt. Hence when an organ,
however abnormal it may be, has been transmitted in approximately the same
condition to many modified descendants, as in the case of the wing of the bat,
it must have existed, according to my theory, for an immense period in nearly
the same state; and thus it comes to be no more variable than any other
structure. It is only in those cases in which the modification has been
comparatively recent and extraordinarily great that we ought to find the generative
variability, as it may be called, still present in a high degree. For in
this case the variability will seldom as yet have been fixed by the continued
selection of the individuals varying in the required manner and degree, and by
the continued rejection of those tending to revert to a former and less
modified condition. The
principle included in these remarks may be extended. It is notorious that
specific characters are more variable than generic. To explain by a simple
example what is meant. If some species in a large genus of plants had blue
flowers and some had red, the colour would be only a specific character, and no
one would be surprised at one of the blue species varying into red, or
conversely; but if all the species had blue flowers, the colour would become a
generic character, and its variation would be a more unusual circumstance. I
have chosen this example because an explanation is not in this case applicable,
which most naturalists would advance, namely, that specific characters are more
variable than generic, because they are taken from parts of less physiological
importance than those commonly used for classing genera. I believe this
explanation is partly, yet only indirectly, true; I shall, however, have to
return to this subject in our chapter on Classification. It would be almost
superfluous to adduce evidence in support of the above statement, that specific
characters are more variable than generic; but I have repeatedly noticed in
works on natural history, that when an author has remarked with surprise that
some important organ or part, which is generally very constant
throughout large groups of species, has differed considerably in
closely-allied species, that it has, also, been variable in the
individuals of some of the species. And this fact shows that a character, which
is generally of generic value, when it sinks in value and becomes only of
specific value, often becomes variable, though its physiological importance may
remain the same. Something of the same kind applies to monstrosities: at least
Is. Geoffroy St. Hilaire seems to entertain no doubt, that the more an organ
normally differs in the different species of the same group, the more subject
it is to individual anomalies. On the
ordinary view of each species having been independently created, why should
that part of the structure, which differs from the same part in other
independently-created species of the same genus, be more variable than those
parts which are closely alike in the several species? I do not see that any
explanation can be given. But on the view of species being only strongly marked
and fixed varieties, we might surely expect to find them still often continuing
to vary in those parts of their structure which have varied within a moderately
recent period, and which have thus come to differ. Or to state the case in
another manner:—the points in which all the species of a genus resemble each
other, and in which they differ from the species of some other genus, are
called generic characters; and these characters in common I attribute to
inheritance from a common progenitor, for it can rarely have happened that
natural selection will have modified several species, fitted to more or less
widely-different habits, in exactly the same manner: and as these so-called
generic characters have been inherited from a remote period, since that period
when the species first branched off from their common progenitor, and
subsequently have not varied or come to differ in any degree, or only in a
slight degree, it is not probable that they should vary at the present day. On
the other hand, the points in which species differ from other species of the
same genus, are called specific characters; and as these specific characters
have varied and come to differ within the period of the branching off of the
species from a common progenitor, it is probable that they should still often
be in some degree variable,—at least more variable than those parts of the
organisation which have for a very long period remained constant. In
connexion with the present subject, I will make only two other remarks. I think
it will be admitted, without my entering on details, that secondary sexual characters
are very variable; I think it also will be admitted that species of the same
group differ from each other more widely in their secondary sexual characters,
than in other parts of their organisation; compare, for instance, the amount of
difference between the males of gallinaceous birds, in which secondary sexual
characters are strongly displayed, with the amount of difference between their
females; and the truth of this proposition will be granted. The cause of the
original variability of secondary sexual characters is not manifest; but we can
see why these characters should not have been rendered as constant and uniform
as other parts of the organisation; for secondary sexual characters have been
accumulated by sexual selection, which is less rigid in its action than
ordinary selection, as it does not entail death, but only gives fewer offspring
to the less favoured males. Whatever the cause may be of the variability of
secondary sexual characters, as they are highly variable, sexual selection will
have had a wide scope for action, and may thus readily have succeeded in giving
to the species of the same group a greater amount of difference in their sexual
characters, than in other parts of their structure. It is a
remarkable fact, that the secondary sexual differences between the two sexes of
the same species are generally displayed in the very same parts of the
organisation in which the different species of the same genus differ from each
other. Of this fact I will give in illustration two instances, the first which
happen to stand on my list; and as the differences in these cases are of a very
unusual nature, the relation can hardly be accidental. The same number of
joints in the tarsi is a character generally common to very large groups of
beetles, but in the Engidæ, as Westwood has remarked, the number varies
greatly; and the number likewise differs in the two sexes of the same species:
again in fossorial hymenoptera, the manner of neuration of the wings is a
character of the highest importance, because common to large groups; but in
certain genera the neuration differs in the different species, and likewise in
the two sexes of the same species. This relation has a clear meaning on my view
of the subject: I look at all the species of the same genus as having as
certainly descended from the same progenitor, as have the two sexes of any one
of the species. Consequently, whatever part of the structure of the common
progenitor, or of its early descendants, became variable; variations of this
part would it is highly probable, be taken advantage of by natural and sexual
selection, in order to fit the several species to their several places in the
economy of nature, and likewise to fit the two sexes of the same species to
each other, or to fit the males and females to different habits of life, or the
males to struggle with other males for the possession of the females. Finally,
then, I conclude that the greater variability of specific characters, or those
which distinguish species from species, than of generic characters, or those
which the species possess in common;—that the frequent extreme variability of
any part which is developed in a species in an extraordinary manner in
comparison with the same part in its congeners; and the not great degree of
variability in a part, however extraordinarily it may be developed, if it be
common to a whole group of species;—that the great variability of secondary
sexual characters, and the great amount of difference in these same characters
between closely allied species;—that secondary sexual and ordinary specific
differences are generally displayed in the same parts of the organisation,—are
all principles closely connected together. All being mainly due to the species
of the same group having descended from a common progenitor, from whom they
have inherited much in common,—to parts which have recently and largely varied
being more likely still to go on varying than parts which have long been
inherited and have not varied,—to natural selection having more or less
completely, according to the lapse of time, overmastered the tendency to
reversion and to further variability,—to sexual selection being less rigid than
ordinary selection,—and to variations in the same parts having been accumulated
by natural and sexual selection, and thus adapted for secondary sexual, and for
ordinary specific purposes. Distinct
species present analogous variations; and a variety of one species often
assumes some of the characters of an allied species, or reverts to some of the
characters of an early progenitor.—These propositions will be most
readily understood by looking to our domestic races. The most distinct breeds
of pigeons, in countries most widely apart, present sub-varieties with reversed
feathers on the head and feathers on the feet,—characters not possessed by the
aboriginal rock-pigeon; these then are analogous variations in two or more
distinct races. The frequent presence of fourteen or even sixteen tail-feathers
in the pouter, may be considered as a variation representing the normal structure
of another race, the fantail. I presume that no one will doubt that all such
analogous variations are due to the several races of the pigeon having
inherited from a common parent the same constitution and tendency to variation,
when acted on by similar unknown influences. In the vegetable kingdom we have a
case of analogous variation, in the enlarged stems, or roots as commonly
called, of the Swedish turnip and Ruta baga, plants which several
botanists rank as varieties produced by cultivation from a common parent: if
this be not so, the case will then be one of analogous variation in two
so-called distinct species; and to these a third may be added, namely, the
common turnip. According to the ordinary view of each species having been
independently created, we should have to attribute this similarity in the
enlarged stems of these three plants, not to the vera causa of
community of descent, and a consequent tendency to vary in a like manner, but
to three separate yet closely related acts of creation. With
pigeons, however, we have another case, namely, the occasional appearance in
all the breeds, of slaty-blue birds with two black bars on the wings, a white
rump, a bar at the end of the tail, with the outer feathers externally edged
near their bases with white. As all these marks are characteristic of the
parent rock-pigeon, I presume that no one will doubt that this is a case of
reversion, and not of a new yet analogous variation appearing in the several
breeds. We may I think confidently come to this conclusion, because, as we have
seen, these coloured marks are eminently liable to appear in the crossed
offspring of two distinct and differently coloured breeds; and in this case
there is nothing in the external conditions of life to cause the reappearance
of the slaty-blue, with the several marks, beyond the influence of the mere act
of crossing on the laws of inheritance. No doubt
it is a very surprising fact that characters should reappear after having been
lost for many, perhaps for hundreds of generations. But when a breed has been
crossed only once by some other breed, the offspring occasionally show a
tendency to revert in character to the foreign breed for many generations—some
say, for a dozen or even a score of generations. After twelve generations, the
proportion of blood, to use a common expression, of any one ancestor, is only 1
in 2048; and yet, as we see, it is generally believed that a tendency to
reversion is retained by this very small proportion of foreign blood. In a
breed which has not been crossed, but in which both parents have lost
some character which their progenitor possessed, the tendency, whether strong
or weak, to reproduce the lost character might be, as was formerly remarked,
for all that we can see to the contrary, transmitted for almost any number of
generations. When a character which has been lost in a breed, reappears after a
great number of generations, the most probable hypothesis is, not that the
offspring suddenly takes after an ancestor some hundred generations distant,
but that in each successive generation there has been a tendency to reproduce
the character in question, which at last, under unknown favourable conditions,
gains an ascendancy. For instance, it is probable that in each generation of
the barb-pigeon, which produces most rarely a blue and black-barred bird, there
has been a tendency in each generation in the plumage to assume this colour.
This view is hypothetical, but could be supported by some facts; and I can see
no more abstract improbability in a tendency to produce any character being
inherited for an endless number of generations, than in quite useless or
rudimentary organs being, as we all know them to be, thus inherited. Indeed, we
may sometimes observe a mere tendency to produce a rudiment inherited: for
instance, in the common snapdragon (Antirrhinum) a rudiment of a fifth stamen
so often appears, that this plant must have an inherited tendency to produce
it. As all the
species of the same genus are supposed, on my theory, to have descended from a
common parent, it might be expected that they would occasionally vary in an
analogous manner; so that a variety of one species would resemble in some of
its characters another species; this other species being on my view only a
well-marked and permanent variety. But characters thus gained would probably be
of an unimportant nature, for the presence of all important characters will be
governed by natural selection, in accordance with the diverse habits of the
species, and will not be left to the mutual action of the conditions of life
and of a similar inherited constitution. It might further be expected that the
species of the same genus would occasionally exhibit reversions to lost
ancestral characters. As, however, we never know the exact character of the
common ancestor of a group, we could not distinguish these two cases: if, for
instance, we did not know that the rock-pigeon was not feather-footed or
turn-crowned, we could not have told, whether these characters in our domestic
breeds were reversions or only analogous variations; but we might have inferred
that the blueness was a case of reversion, from the number of the markings,
which are correlated with the blue tint, and which it does not appear probable
would all appear together from simple variation. More especially we might have
inferred this, from the blue colour and marks so often appearing when distinct
breeds of diverse colours are crossed. Hence, though under nature it must
generally be left doubtful, what cases are reversions to an anciently existing
character, and what are new but analogous variations, yet we ought, on my
theory, sometimes to find the varying offspring of a species assuming
characters (either from reversion or from analogous variation) which already
occur in some other members of the same group. And this undoubtedly is the case
in nature. A
considerable part of the difficulty in recognising a variable species in our
systematic works, is due to its varieties mocking, as it were, some of the
other species of the same genus. A considerable catalogue, also, could be given
of forms intermediate between two other forms, which themselves must be
doubtfully ranked as either varieties or species; and this shows, unless all
these forms be considered as independently created species, that the one in
varying has assumed some of the characters of the other, so as to produce the
intermediate form. But the best evidence is afforded by parts or organs of an
important and uniform nature occasionally varying so as to acquire, in some
degree, the character of the same part or organ in an allied species. I have
collected a long list of such cases; but here, as before, I lie under a great
disadvantage in not being able to give them. I can only repeat that such cases
certainly do occur, and seem to me very remarkable. I will,
however, give one curious and complex case, not indeed as affecting any
important character, but from occurring in several species of the same genus,
partly under domestication and partly under nature. It is a case apparently of
reversion. The ass not rarely has very distinct transverse bars on its legs,
like those on the legs of a zebra: it has been asserted that these are plainest
in the foal, and from inquiries which I have made, I believe this to be true.
It has also been asserted that the stripe on each shoulder is sometimes double.
The shoulder stripe is certainly very variable in length and outline. A white
ass, but not an albino, has been described without either spinal or
shoulder-stripe; and these stripes are sometimes very obscure, or actually
quite lost, in dark-coloured asses. The koulan of Pallas is said to have been
seen with a double shoulder-stripe. The hemionus has no shoulder-stripe; but
traces of it, as stated by Mr. Blyth and others, occasionally appear: and I
have been informed by Colonel Poole that the foals of this species are
generally striped on the legs, and faintly on the shoulder. The quagga, though
so plainly barred like a zebra over the body, is without bars on the legs; but
Dr. Gray has figured one specimen with very distinct zebra-like bars on the
hocks. With
respect to the horse, I have collected cases in England of the spinal stripe in
horses of the most distinct breeds, and of all colours; transverse
bars on the legs are not rare in duns, mouse-duns, and in one instance in a
chestnut: a faint shoulder-stripe may sometimes be seen in duns, and I have
seen a trace in a bay horse. My son made a careful examination and sketch for
me of a dun Belgian cart-horse with a double stripe on each shoulder and with
leg-stripes; and a man, whom I can implicitly trust, has examined for me a
small dun Welch pony with three short parallel stripes on each
shoulder. In the
north-west part of India the Kattywar breed of horses is so generally striped,
that, as I hear from Colonel Poole, who examined the breed for the Indian
Government, a horse without stripes is not considered as purely-bred. The spine
is always striped; the legs are generally barred; and the shoulder-stripe,
which is sometimes double and sometimes treble, is common; the side of the
face, moreover, is sometimes striped. The stripes are plainest in the foal; and
sometimes quite disappear in old horses. Colonel Poole has seen both gray and
bay Kattywar horses striped when first foaled. I have, also, reason to suspect,
from information given me by Mr. W. W. Edwards, that with the English
race-horse the spinal stripe is much commoner in the foal than in the
full-grown animal. Without here entering on further details, I may state that I
have collected cases of leg and shoulder stripes in horses of very different
breeds, in various countries from Britain to Eastern China; and from Norway in
the north to the Malay Archipelago in the south. In all parts of the world
these stripes occur far oftenest in duns and mouse-duns; by the term dun a
large range of colour is included, from one between brown and black to a close
approach to cream-colour. I am aware
that Colonel Hamilton Smith, who has written on this subject, believes that the
several breeds of the horse have descended from several aboriginal species—one
of which, the dun, was striped; and that the above-described appearances are
all due to ancient crosses with the dun stock. But I am not at all satisfied
with this theory, and should be loth to apply it to breeds so distinct as the
heavy Belgian cart-horse, Welch ponies, cobs, the lanky Kattywar race, &c.,
inhabiting the most distant parts of the world. Now let us
turn to the effects of crossing the several species of the horse-genus. Rollin
asserts, that the common mule from the ass and horse is particularly apt to
have bars on its legs. I once saw a mule with its legs so much striped that any
one at first would have thought that it must have been the product of a zebra;
and Mr. W. C. Martin, in his excellent treatise on the horse, has given a
figure of a similar mule. In four coloured drawings, which I have seen, of
hybrids between the ass and zebra, the legs were much more plainly barred than
the rest of the body; and in one of them there was a double shoulder-stripe. In
Lord Moreton’s famous hybrid from a chestnut mare and male quagga, the hybrid,
and even the pure offspring subsequently produced from the mare by a black
Arabian sire, were much more plainly barred across the legs than is even the
pure quagga. Lastly, and this is another most remarkable case, a hybrid has
been figured by Dr. Gray (and he informs me that he knows of a second case)
from the ass and the hemionus; and this hybrid, though the ass seldom has
stripes on its legs and the hemionus has none and has not even a
shoulder-stripe, nevertheless had all four legs barred, and had three short
shoulder-stripes, like those on the dun Welch pony, and even had some
zebra-like stripes on the sides of its face. With respect to this last fact, I
was so convinced that not even a stripe of colour appears from what would
commonly be called an accident, that I was led solely from the occurrence of
the face-stripes on this hybrid from the ass and hemionus, to ask Colonel Poole
whether such face-stripes ever occur in the eminently striped Kattywar breed of
horses, and was, as we have seen, answered in the affirmative. What now
are we to say to these several facts? We see several very distinct species of
the horse-genus becoming, by simple variation, striped on the legs like a
zebra, or striped on the shoulders like an ass. In the horse we see this
tendency strong whenever a dun tint appears—a tint which approaches to that of
the general colouring of the other species of the genus. The appearance of the
stripes is not accompanied by any change of form or by any other new character.
We see this tendency to become striped most strongly displayed in hybrids from
between several of the most distinct species. Now observe the case of the
several breeds of pigeons: they are descended from a pigeon (including two or
three sub-species or geographical races) of a bluish colour, with certain bars
and other marks; and when any breed assumes by simple variation a bluish tint,
these bars and other marks invariably reappear; but without any other change of
form or character. When the oldest and truest breeds of various colours are
crossed, we see a strong tendency for the blue tint and bars and marks to
reappear in the mongrels. I have stated that the most probable hypothesis to account
for the reappearance of very ancient characters, is—that there is a tendency
in the young of each successive generation to produce the long-lost character,
and that this tendency, from unknown causes, sometimes prevails. And we have
just seen that in several species of the horse-genus the stripes are either
plainer or appear more commonly in the young than in the old. Call the breeds
of pigeons, some of which have bred true for centuries, species; and how
exactly parallel is the case with that of the species of the horse-genus! For
myself, I venture confidently to look back thousands on thousands of
generations, and I see an animal striped like a zebra, but perhaps otherwise
very differently constructed, the common parent of our domestic horse, whether or
not it be descended from one or more wild stocks, of the ass, the hemionus,
quagga, and zebra. He who
believes that each equine species was independently created, will, I presume,
assert that each species has been created with a tendency to vary, both under
nature and under domestication, in this particular manner, so as often to
become striped like other species of the genus; and that each has been created
with a strong tendency, when crossed with species inhabiting distant quarters
of the world, to produce hybrids resembling in their stripes, not their own
parents, but other species of the genus. To admit this view is, as it seems to
me, to reject a real for an unreal, or at least for an unknown, cause. It makes
the works of God a mere mockery and deception; I would almost as soon believe
with the old and ignorant cosmogonists, that fossil shells had never lived, but
had been created in stone so as to mock the shells now living on the sea-shore. Summary.—Our
ignorance of the laws of variation is profound. Not in one case out of a
hundred can we pretend to assign any reason why this or that part differs, more
or less, from the same part in the parents. But whenever we have the means of
instituting a comparison, the same laws appear to have acted in producing the
lesser differences between varieties of the same species, and the greater
differences between species of the same genus. The external conditions of life,
as climate and food, &c., seem to have induced some slight modifications.
Habit in producing constitutional differences, and use in strengthening, and
disuse in weakening and diminishing organs, seem to have been more potent in
their effects. Homologous parts tend to vary in the same way, and homologous
parts tend to cohere. Modifications in hard parts and in external parts
sometimes affect softer and internal parts. When one part is largely developed,
perhaps it tends to draw nourishment from the adjoining parts; and every part
of the structure which can be saved without detriment to the individual, will
be saved. Changes of structure at an early age will generally affect parts
subsequently developed; and there are very many other correlations of growth,
the nature of which we are utterly unable to understand. Multiple parts are
variable in number and in structure, perhaps arising from such parts not having
been closely specialised to any particular function, so that their
modifications have not been closely checked by natural selection. It is
probably from this same cause that organic beings low in the scale of nature
are more variable than those which have their whole organisation more
specialised, and are higher in the scale. Rudimentary organs, from being
useless, will be disregarded by natural selection, and hence probably are
variable. Specific characters—that is, the characters which have come to differ
since the several species of the same genus branched off from a common
parent—are more variable than generic characters, or those which have long been
inherited, and have not differed within this same period. In these remarks we
have referred to special parts or organs being still variable, because they
have recently varied and thus come to differ; but we have also seen in the
second Chapter that the same principle applies to the whole individual; for in
a district where many species of any genus are found—that is, where there has
been much former variation and differentiation, or where the manufactory of new
specific forms has been actively at work—there, on an average, we now find most
varieties or incipient species. Secondary sexual characters are highly
variable, and such characters differ much in the species of the same group.
Variability in the same parts of the organisation has generally been taken
advantage of in giving secondary sexual differences to the sexes of the same
species, and specific differences to the several species of the same genus. Any
part or organ developed to an extraordinary size or in an extraordinary manner,
in comparison with the same part or organ in the allied species, must have gone
through an extraordinary amount of modification since the genus arose; and thus
we can understand why it should often still be variable in a much higher degree
than other parts; for variation is a long-continued and slow process, and natural
selection will in such cases not as yet have had time to overcome the tendency
to further variability and to reversion to a less modified state. But when a
species with any extraordinarily-developed organ has become the parent of many
modified descendants—which on my view must be a very slow process, requiring a
long lapse of time—in this case, natural selection may readily have succeeded
in giving a fixed character to the organ, in however extraordinary a manner it
may be developed. Species inheriting nearly the same constitution from a common
parent and exposed to similar influences will naturally tend to present
analogous variations, and these same species may occasionally revert to some of
the characters of their ancient progenitors. Although new and important
modifications may not arise from reversion and analogous variation, such
modifications will add to the beautiful and harmonious diversity of nature. Whatever
the cause may be of each slight difference in the offspring from their
parents—and a cause for each must exist—it is the steady accumulation, through
natural selection, of such differences, when beneficial to the individual, that
gives rise to all the more important modifications of structure, by which the
innumerable beings on the face of this earth are enabled to struggle with each
other, and the best adapted to survive. |