Web
and Book design,
Copyright, Kellscraft Studio 1999-2006 (Return
to Web
Text-ures)
|
Click
Here to return to Birds in the Bush Content Page Return to the Previous Chapter |
(HOME) |
WHY do birds sing? Has their music a meaning, or is it all a matter of blind impulse? Some bright morning in March, as you go out-of-doors, you are greeted by the notes of the first robin. Perched in a leafless tree, there he sits, facing the sun like a genuine fire-worshiper, and singing as though he would pour out his very soul. What is he thinking about? What spirit possesses him? It is easy
to ask
questions until the simplest matter comes to seem, what at bottom it
really is,
a thing altogether mysterious; but if our robin could understand us, he
would,
likely enough, reply: — “Why do you talk in this way, as if it were
something requiring explanation that a bird should sing? You seem to
have
forgotten that everybody sings, or almost everybody. Think of the
insects, —
the bees and the crickets and the locusts, to say nothing of your
intimate
friends, the mosquitoes? Think, too, of the frogs and the hylas! If
these
cold-blooded, low-lived creatures, after sleeping all winter in the mud,1
are free to make so much use of their voices, surely a bird of the air
may sing
his unobtrusive song without being cross-examined concerning the
purpose of it.
Why do the mice sing, and the monkeys, and the woodchucks? Indeed, sir,
— if
one may be so bold, — why do you sing, yourself?” This
matter-of-fact
Darwinism need not frighten us. It will do us no harm to remember, now
and
then, the hole of the pit whence we were digged; and besides, as far as
any
relationship between us and the birds is concerned, it is doubtful
whether we
are the party to complain. But
avoiding
“genealogies and contentions,” and taking up the question with which we
began,
we may safely say that birds sing, sometimes to gratify an innate love
for
sweet sounds; sometimes to win a mate, or to tell their love to a mate
already
won; sometimes as practice, with a view to self-improvement; and
sometimes for
no better reason than the poet’s, — “I do but sing because I must.” In
general,
they sing for joy; and their joy, of course, has various causes. For one
thing, they
are very sensitive to the weather. With them, as with us, sunlight and
a genial
warmth go to produce serenity. A bright summer-like day, late in
October, or
even in November, will set the smaller birds to singing, and the grouse
to
drumming. I heard a robin venturing a little song on the 25th of last
December;
but that, for aught I know, was a Christmas carol. No matter what the
season,
you will not hear a great deal of bird music during a high wind; and if
you are
caught in the woods by a sudden shower in May or June, and are not too
much
taken up with thoughts of your own condition, you will hardly fail to
notice
the instant silence which falls upon the woods with the rain. Birds,
however,
are more or less inconsistent (that is a part of their likeness to us),
and
sometimes sing most freely when the sky is overcast. But their
highest
joys are by no means dependent upon the moods of the weather. A
comfortable
state of mind is not to be contemned, but beings who are capable of
deep and
passionate affection recognize a difference between comfort and
ecstasy. And
the peculiar glory of birds is just here, in the all-consuming fervor
of their
love. It would be commonplace to call them models of conjugal and
parental
faithfulness. With a few exceptions (and these, it is a pleasure to
add, not
singers), the very least of them is literally faithful unto death. Here
and
there, in the notes of some collector, we are told of a difficulty he
has had
in securing a coveted specimen: the tiny creature, whose mate had been
already
“collected,” would persist in hovering so closely about the invader’s
head that
it was impossible to shoot him without spoiling him for the cabinet by
blowing
him to pieces! Need there
be any
mystery about the singing of such a lover? Is it surprising if at times
he is
so enraptured that he can no longer sit tamely on the branch, but must
dart
into the air, and go circling round and round, caroling as he flies? So far as
song is
the voice of emotion, it will of necessity vary with the emotion; and
every one
who has ears must have heard once in a while bird music of quite
unusual
fervor. For example, I have often seen the least flycatcher (a very
unromantic-looking body, surely) when he was almost beside himself;
flying in a
circle, and repeating breathlessly his emphatic chebec. And once I found a wood
pewee in a somewhat similar
mood. He was more quiet than the least flycatcher; but he too sang on
the wing,
and I have never heard notes which seemed more expressive of happiness.
Many of
them were entirely new and strange, although the familiar pewee was introduced among the
rest. As I
listened, I felt it to be an occasion for thankfulness that the
delighted
creature had never studied anatomy, and did not know that the structure
of his
throat made it improper for him to sing. In this connection, also, I
recall a
cardinal grosbeak, whom I heard several years ago, on the bank of the
Potomac
River. An old soldier had taken me to visit the Great Falls, and as we
were
clambering over the rocks this grosbeak began to sing; and soon,
without any
hint from me, and without knowing who the invisible musician was, my
companion
remarked upon the uncommon beauty of the song. The cardinal is always a
great
singer, having a voice which, as European writers say, is almost equal
to the
nightingale’s; but in this case the more stirring, martial quality of
the
strain had given place to an exquisite mellowness, as if it were, what
I have
no doubt it was, a song of love. Every kind
of bird
has notes of its own, so that a thoroughly practiced ear would be able
to
discriminate the different species with nearly as much certainty as
Professor
Baird would feel after an examination of the anatomy and plumage. Still
this
strong specific resemblance is far from being a dead uniformity. Aside
from the
fact, already mentioned, that the characteristic strain is sometimes
given with
extraordinary sweetness and emphasis, there are often to be detected
variations
of a more formal character. This is noticeably true of robins. It may
almost be
said that no two of them sing alike; while now and then their vagaries
are
conspicuous enough to attract general attention. One who was my
neighbor last
year interjected into his song a series of four or five most exact
imitations
of the peep of a chicken. When I first heard this performance, I was in
company
with two friends, both of whom noticed and laughed at it; and some days
afterwards I visited the spot again, and found the bird still
rehearsing the
same ridiculous medley. I conjectured that he had been brought up near
a
hen-coop, and, moreover, had been so unfortunate as to lose his father
before
his notes had become thoroughly fixed; and then, being compelled to
finish his
musical education by himself, had taken a fancy to practice these
chicken
calls. This guess may not have been correct. All I can affirm is that
he sang
exactly as he might have been expected to do, on that supposition; but
certainly the resemblance seemed too close to be accidental. The
variations of
the wood thrush are fully as striking as those of the robin, and
sometimes it
is impossible not to feel that the artist is making a deliberate effort
to do
something out of the ordinary course, something better than he has ever
done
before. Now and then he prefaces his proper song with many
disconnected,
extremely staccato
notes,
following each other at very distant and unexpected intervals of pitch.
It is
this, I conclude, which is meant by some writer (who it is I cannot now
remember) when he criticises the wood thrush for spending too much time
in
tuning his instrument. But the fault is the critic’s, I think; to my
ear these
preliminaries sound rather like the recitative which goes before the
grand aria. Still
another
musician who delights to take liberties with his score is the towhee
bunting,
or chewink. Indeed, he carries the matter so far that sometimes it
seems almost
as if he suspected the proximity of some self-conceited ornithologist,
and were
determined, if possible, to make a fool of him. And for my part, being
neither
self-conceited nor an ornithologist, I am willing to confess that I
have once
or twice been so badly deceived that now the mere sight of this Pipilo is, so to speak, a means
of grace
to me. One more
of these
innovators (these heretics, as they are most likely called by their
more
conservative brethren) is the field sparrow, better known as Spizella pusilla. His usual
song consists
of a simple line of notes, beginning leisurely, but growing shorter and
more
rapid to the close. The voice is so smooth and sweet, and the
acceleration so
well managed, that, although the whole is commonly a strict monotone,
the
effect is not in the least monotonous. This song I once heard rendered
in
reverse order, with a result so strange that I did not suspect the
identity of
the author till I had crept up within sight of him. Another of these
sparrows,
who has passed the last two seasons in my neighborhood, habitually
doubles the
measure; going through it in the usual way, and then, just as you
expect him to
conclude, catching it up again, Da
capo. But birds
like
these are quite outdone by such species as the song sparrow, the
white-eyed
vireo, and the Western meadow-lark, species of which we may say that
each
individual bird has a whole repertory of songs at his command. The song
sparrow, who is the best known of the three, will repeat one melody
perhaps a
dozen times, then change it for a second, and in turn leave that for a
third;
as if he were singing hymns of twelve or fifteen stanzas each, and set
each
hymn to its appropriate tune. It is something well worth listening to,
common
though it is, and may easily suggest a number of questions about the
origin and
meaning of bird music. The
white-eyed
vireo is a singer of astonishing spirit, and his sudden changes from
one theme
to another are sometimes almost startling. He is a skillful
ventriloquist,
also, and I remember one in particular who outwitted me completely. He
was
rehearsing a well-known strain, but at the end there came up from the
bushes
underneath a querulous call. At first I took it for granted that some
other
bird was in the underbrush; but the note was repeated too many times,
and came
in too exactly on the beat. I have no
personal
acquaintance with the Western meadow-lark, but no less than twenty-six
of his
songs have been printed in musical notation, and these are said to be
by no
means all.2 Others of
our birds
have similar gifts, though no others, so far as I know, are quite so
versatile
as these three. Several of the warblers, for example, have attained to
more
than one set song, notwithstanding the deservedly small reputation of
this
misnamed family. I have myself heard the golden-crowned thrush, the
black-throated green warbler, the black-throated blue, the
yellow-rumped, and
the chestnut-sided, sing two melodies each, while the blue
golden-winged has at
least three; and this, of course, without making anything of slight
variations
such as all birds are more or less accustomed to indulge in. The best
of the
three songs of the blue golden-wing I have never heard except on one
occasion,
but then it was repeated for half an hour under my very eyes. It bore
no
resemblance to the common dsee,
dsee, dsee,
of the species, and would appear to be seldom used; for not only have I
never
heard it since, but none of the writers seem ever to have heard it at
all.
However, I still keep a careful description of it, which I took down on
the
spot, and which I expect some future golden-wing to verify. But the
most
celebrated of the warblers in this regard is the golden-crowned thrush,
otherwise called the oven-bird and the wood wagtail. His ordinary
effort is one
of the noisiest, least melodious, and most incessant sounds to be heard
in our
woods. His song is
another
matter. For that he takes to the air (usually starting from a tree-top,
although I have seen him rise from the ground), whence, after a
preliminary chip, chip,
he lets falls a hurried flood
of notes, in the midst of which can usually be distinguished his
familiar weeehee, weeehee, weechee.
It is nothing wonderful
that he should sing on the wing, — many other birds do the same, and
very much
better than he; but he is singular in that he strictly reserves his
aerial
music for late in the afternoon. I have heard it as early as three
o’clock, but
never before that, and it is most common about sunset. Writers speak of
it as
limited to the season of courtship; but I have heard it almost daily
till near
the end of July, and once, for my special benefit, perhaps, it was
given in
full — and repeated— on the first day of September. But who taught the
little
creature to do this, — to sing one song in the forenoon, perched upon a
twig,
and to keep another for afternoon, singing that invariably on the wing?
and
what difference is there between the two in the mind of the singer?3
It is an
indiscretion ever to say of a bird that he has only such and such
notes. You
may have been his friend for years, but the next time you go into the
woods he
will likely enough put you to shame by singing something not so much as
hinted
at in your description. I thought I knew the song of the yellow-rumped
warbler,
having listened to it many times, — a slight and rather characterless
thing,
nowise remarkable. But coming down Mount Willard one day in June, I
heard a
warbler’s song which brought me to a sudden halt. It was new and
beautiful, —
more beautiful, it seemed at the moment, than any warbler’s song I had
ever
heard. What could it be? A little patient waiting (while the
black-flies and
mosquitoes “came upon me to eat up my flesh”), and the wonderful
stranger
appeared in full view, — my old acquaintance, the yellow-rumped
warbler. With all
this
strong tendency on the part of birds to vary their music, how is it
that there
is still such a degree of uniformity, so that, as we have said, every
species
may be recognized by its notes? Why does every red-eyed vireo sing in
one way,
and every white-eyed vireo in another? Who teaches the young chipper to
trill,
and the young linnet to warble? In short, how do birds come by their
music? Is
it all a matter of instinct, inherited habit, or do they learn it? The
answer
appears to be that birds sing as children talk, by simple imitation.
Nobody
imagines that the infant is born with a language printed upon his
brain. The
father and mother may never have known a word of any tongue except the
English,
but if the child is brought up to hear only Chinese, he will infallibly
speak
that, and nothing else. And careful experiments have shown the same to
be true
of birds.4 Taken from the nest just after they leave the
shell, they
invariably sing, not their own so-called natural song, but the song of
their
foster-parents; provided, of course, that this is not anything beyond
their
physical capacity. The notorious house sparrow (our “English” sparrow),
in his
wild or semi-domesticated state, never makes a musical sound; but if he
is
taken in hand early enough, he may be taught to sing, so it is said,
nearly as
well as the canary. Bechstein relates that a Paris clergyman had two of
these
sparrows whom he had trained to speak, and, among other things, to
recite
several of the shorter commandments; and the narrative goes on to say
that it
was sometimes very comical, when the pair were disputing over their
food, to
hear one gravely admonish the other, “Thou shalt not steal!” It would
be
interesting to know why creatures thus gifted do not sing of their own
motion.
With their amiability and sweet peaceableness they ought to be caroling
the
whole year round. This
question of
the transmission of songs from one generation to another is, of course,
a part
of the general subject of animal intelligence, a subject much discussed
in
these days on account of its bearing upon the modern doctrine
concerning the
relation of man to the inferior orders. We have
nothing to
do with such a theme, but it may not be out of place to suggest to
preachers
and moralists that here is a striking and unhackneyed illustration of
the force
of early training. Birds sing by imitation, it is true, but as a rule
they
imitate only the notes which they hear during the first few weeks after
they
are hatched. One of Mr. Barring-ton’s linnets, for example, after being
educated under a titlark, was put into a room with two birds of his own
species, where he heard them sing freely every day for three months. He
made no
attempt to learn anything from them, however, but kept on practicing
what the
titlark had taught him, quite unconscious of anything singular or
unpatriotic
in such a course. This law, that impressions received during the
immaturity of
the powers become the unalterable habit of the after life, is perhaps
the most
momentous of all the laws in whose power we find ourselves. Sometimes
we are
tempted to call it cruel. But if it were annulled, this would be a
strange
world. What a hurly-burly we should have among the birds! There would
be no
more telling them by their notes. Thrushes and jays, wrens and
chickadees,
finches and warblers, all would be singing one grand medley. Between
these two
opposing tendencies, one urging to variation, the other to permanence
(for
Nature herself is half radical, half conservative), the language of
birds has
grown from rude beginnings to its present beautiful diversity; and
whoever
lives a century of millenniums hence will listen to music such as we in
this
day can only dream of. Inappreciably but ceaselessly the work goes on.
Here and
there is born a master-singer, a feathered genius, and every generation
makes
its own addition to the glorious inheritance. It may be
doubted
whether there is any real connection between moral character and the
possession
of wings. Nevertheless there has long been a popular feeling that some
such
congruity does exist; and certainly it seems unreasonable to suppose
that
creatures who are able to soar at will into the heavens should be
without other
equally angelic attributes. But, be that as it may, our friends, the
birds, do
undeniably set us a good example in several respects. To mention only
one, how
becoming is their observance of morning and evening song In spite
of their
industrious spirit (and few of us labor more hours daily), neither
their first
nor their last thoughts are given to the question, What shall we eat,
and what
shall we drink? Possibly their habit of saluting the rising and setting
sun may
be thought to favor the theory that the worship of the god of day was
the
original religion. I know nothing about that. But it would be a sad
change if
the birds, declining from their present beautiful custom, were to sleep
and
work, work and sleep, with no holy hour between, as is too much the
case with
the being who, according to his own pharisaic notion, is the only
religious
animal. In the
season,
however, the woods are by no means silent, even at noonday. Many
species (such
as the vireos and warblers, who get their living amid the foliage of
trees)
sing as they work; while the thrushes and others, who keep business and
pleasure more distinct, are often too happy to go many hours together
without a
hymn. I have even seen robins singing without quitting the turf; but
that is
rather unusual, for somehow birds have come to feel that they must get
away
from the ground when the lyrical mood is upon them. This may be a thing
of
sentiment (for is not language full of uncomplimentary allusions to
earth and
earthliness?), but more likely it is prudential. The gift of song is no
doubt a
dangerous blessing to creatures who have so many enemies, and we can
readily
believe that they have found it safer to be up where they can look
about them
while thus publishing their whereabouts. A very
interesting
exception to this rule is the savanna sparrow, who sings habitually
from the
ground. But even he shares the common feeling, and stretches himself to
his
full height with an earnestness which is almost laughable, in view of
the
result; for his notes are hardly louder than a cricket’s chirp.
Probably he has
fallen into this lowly habit from living in meadows and salt marshes,
where
bushes and trees are not readily to be come at; and it is worth
noticing that,
in the case of the skylark and the white-winged blackbird, the same
conditions
have led to a result precisely opposite. The sparrow, we may presume,
was
originally of a humble disposition, and when nothing better offered
itself for
a singing-perch easily grew accustomed to standing upon a stone or a
little
lump of earth; and this practice, long persisted in, naturally had the
effect
to lessen the loudness of his voice. The skylark, on the other hand,
when he
did not readily find a tree-top, said to himself, “Never mind! I have a
pair of
wings.” And so the lark is famous, while the sparrow remains
unheard-of, and is
even mistaken for a grasshopper. How true
it is that
the very things which dishearten one nature and break it down, only
help
another to find out what it was made for! If you would foretell the
development, either of a bird or of a man, it is not enough to know his
environment, you must know also what there is in him. We have
possibly
made too much of the savanna sparrow’s innocent eccentricity. He fills
his
place, and fills it well; and who knows but that he may yet outshine
the
skylark? There is a promise, I believe, for those who humble
themselves. But
what shall be said of species which do not even try to sing, and that,
notwithstanding they have all the structural peculiarities of singing
birds,
and must, almost certainly, have come from ancestors who were singers?
We have
already mentioned the house sparrow, whose defect is the more
mysterious on
account of his belonging to so highly musical a family. But he was never accused of not
being noisy
enough, while we have one bird who, though he is classed with the
oscines,
passes his life in almost unbroken silence. Of course I refer to the
waxwing,
or cedar-bird, whose faint, sibilant whisper can scarcely be thought to
contradict the foregoing description. By what
strange
freak he has lapsed into this ghostly habit, nobody knows. I make no
account of
the insinuation that he gave up music because it hindered his success
in
cherry-stealing. He likes cherries, it is true; and who can blame him?
But he
would need to work hard to steal more than does that indefatigable
songster,
the robin. I feel sure he has some better reason than this for his
Quakerish
conduct. But, however he came by his stillness, it is likely that by
this time
he plumes himself upon it. Silence is golden, he thinks, the supreme
result of
the highest esthetic culture. Those loud creatures, the thrushes and
finches!
What a vulgar set they are, to be sure, the more’s the pity! Certainly
if he
does not reason in some such way, bird nature is not so human as we
have given
it credit for being. Besides, the waxwing has an uncommon appreciation
of the
decorous; at least, we must think so if we are able to credit a story
of
Nuttall’s. He declares that a Boston gentleman, whose name he gives,
saw one of
a company of these birds capture an insect, and offer it to his
neighbor; he,
however, delicately declined the dainty bit, and it was offered to the
next,
who, in turn, was equally polite; and the morsel actually passed back
and forth
along the line, till, finally, one of the flock was persuaded to eat
it. I have
never seen
anything equal to this; but one day, happening to stop under a low
cedar, I
discovered right over my head a waxwing’s nest with the mother-bird
sitting
upon it, while her mate was perched beside her on the branch. He was
barely out
of my reach, but he did not move a muscle; and although he uttered no
sound,
his behavior said as plainly as possible, “What do you expect to do here? Don’t you see I am standing guard over this
nest?” I
should be ashamed not to be able to add that I respected his dignity
and
courage, and left him and his castle unmolested. Observations
so discursive as these can hardly be finished; they must break off
abruptly, or
else go on forever. Let us make an end, therefore, with expressing our
hope
that the cedar-bird, already so handsome and chivalrous, will yet take
to
himself a song; one sweet and original, worthy to go with his soft
satin coat,
his ornaments of sealing-wax, and his magnificent top-knot. Let him do
that,
and he shall always be made welcome; yes, even though he come in force
and in
cherry-time. 1 There is no
Historic-Genealogical
Society among the birds, and the robin is not aware that his own remote
ancestors were reptiles. If he were, he would hardly speak so
disrespectfully
of these batrachians. 2 Mr. C. N. Allen, in Bulletin of the Nuttall Ornithological Club,
July, 1881. 3 Since this paper was
written I have
three times heard the wood wagtail’s true song in the morning, — but in
neither
case was the bird in the air. See p. 284. 4 See the paper of
Daines Barrington
in Philosophical Transactions
for
1773; also, Darwin’s Descent of
Man,
and Wallace’s Natural Selection.
|